The recognition of nasal-stop distinction in adverse listening conditions

Document Type : Research

Author

Faculty of Information Science and Technology Research Institute of Ira

Abstract

The automatic recognition of Persian numerals [sefr-se] “zero–three”, [do-noh] “two-nine” and [haft-haʃt] “seven-eight” is considered as a challenge for speech recognition systems. Mahmoodzadeh and Bahrani (2014) found that the acoustic reduction of telephone speech triggers lexical ambiguity for the automatic recognition of [sefr-se] pair. The numeral [sefr] is produced with the deletion of [r] at word final position and the weak labial friction of [f] is masked by the channel noise, which results in an increase of acoustic similarity between [sefr] and [se]. 
     The automated recognition of phonological distinctions can be improved by using perceptual cues which listeners apply for the natural recognition of sounds in adverse listening conditions such as telephone speech or noisy environment. In this research for discovering efficient perceptual cues responsible for distinction of numerals [do]-[noh] “two-nine” in both natural and telephone speech, perception tests were used. The acoustic signal of [noh] shows a weak or practically deleted final [h], which is not audible and recognizable from background noise. Therefore, the acoustic differences of nasal-stop distinction and the co-articulatory effects of nasal on the following vowel play an important role in [do-no] recognition.
     Results show that the acoustic landmarks, nasal murmur and nasal-vowel transitions affected by various sources of signal disruption, undergo phonetic reduction, which leads to perceptual similarity of [do-noh] pair and ultimately listeners` lexical ambiguity. In optimal listening condition, natural recognition of [n] based on nasal murmur is about 40% which increases to 96% after the addition of 10ms of the beginning of vowel [o]. However in telephone speech, natural recognition of [n] based on murmur is about 29% and after the addition of 10ms of vowel [o] transitions rises to just about 48%. According to the outcomes, nasal murmur is not an influential perceptual cue for the recognition of [n] in both optimal and adverse listening conditions; however recognition rises significantly after adding only 10 ms of the beginning of following vowel. It is likely that listeners’ lexical confusion and uncertainty due to lack and reduction of phonetic information reinforced by listeners’ perceptual bias toward unmarked consonant, give rise to [d] responses and failure of [n] identification in adverse listening conditions. The automatic recognition of these words should be done by capturing further temporal and spectral information from neighboring vowel transitions.
 
 

Keywords


Bybee, J. L. (2001). Phonology and language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Chang, S. S., Plauche, M. C. and Ohala, J. J. (2001). Markedness and consonant confusion asymmetries. In E. Hume and K. Johnson (Eds.), The Role of Speech Perception in Phonology (pp. 79-101). San Diego. Academic press.
Davis, M. H., Johnsrude, I. S., Hervais-Adelman, A., Taylor. K., & McGettigan, C. (2005). Lexical information drives perceptual learning of distorted speech: Evidence from the comprehension of noise-vocoded sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 222- 241.
Ernestus M., Baayen, H., & Schreuder, R. (2002). The recognition of reduced word forms. Brain and Language. 81, 162- 173.
Fujimura, O. (1962). Analysis of nasal consonants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 34 (12), 1865–1875.
Goldstone, R. L. (1998). Perceptual learning. Annual Review of Psychology. 49, 585-612.
Johnson, K. (2015). Acoustic and auditory phonetics. Translated by Z. Mahmoodzadeh and R. Rouhparvar. Tehran: Chapar publication [in Persian].
Kingston, J., & Shinya, T. (2003). Markedness asymmetries in place perception in consonant clusters. In M. J. Solé, D. Recasens, and J. Romero; Barcelona.International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 399-402). Barcelona, Spain.
Mahmoodzadeh, Z., & Bahrani, M. (2014). A case study of phonetic reduction in Persian telephonic numerals. Quarterly Journal of Language Studies Alzahra University, 6 (13), 110-134 [in Persian].
Mattys, S. L., M. H. Davis, A. R. Bradlow & S. K. Scott (2012). Speech recognition in adverse conditions: A review. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27 (7-8), 953-978.
Pruthi, T., & Espy-Wilson, C.  Y. (2004). Acoustic parameters for automatic detection of nasal manner. Speech Communication, 43, 225–239.
Recasens, D. (1982). Place cues for nasal consonants with special reference to Catalan. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 73(4), 1346-1353.
Samareh, Y. (2006). Phonology of Persian language. Tehran: University Publication Center [in Persian].
Weinstein, C. J., McCandless, S. S., Mondshein, L. F., & Zue, V. W. (1975). A system for acoustic-phonetic analysis of continuous speech. IEEE Transactions on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing, 23, 54–67.