Document Type : Research

Author

Department of Linguistics, Allameh Tabataba'i University, Faculty of Persian literature and Foreign Languages, Tehran, Iran

Abstract

A narrative discourse is a discourse that is an account of events, usually in the past, that employs verbs of speech, motion, and action to describe a series of events that are contingent one on another, and that typically focuses on one or more performers of actions. Narrative discourse has been of intense interest to researchers. The ability to produce coherent discourse as an important component of child developmental knowledge during language acquisition is an essential part of language knowledge which child should acquire. The aims of the present research are to investigate the ability of monolingual Persian-speaking children in maintaining narrative coherence and also to compare the findings with other studies in order to acknowledge previous findings based on analysis of causal network. To this aim, thirty 5 to 6-year-old children (mean age= 5 years and 6 months), thirty 9 to10-year-old children (mean age= 9 years and 6 months) were selected among 100 children. Thirty 20-25 male adults (mean age=23 years and 8 months) participated in the study. All participants were typically developing children and had no learning disabilities or speech or hearing problems and were of the same socio-economic status and verbal intelligence. Moreover, children were also controlled for their productive and receptive language abilities and only those children with the same language abilities were selected. These groups were encouraged to narrate two picture story books namely “Frog, Where are you?” and “Frog”.  The first one is about a boy who loses his rabbit and is engaged in an adventure to find it. This story is a reliable tool to investigate narrative abilities of children. The second one (i.e. “Frog”) was used to control the content of the story. This story consisted of a series of complex events which enables the narrator to produce various connections between those events. Therefore, it is suitable for investigating how different events are connected to each other. The interviews were carried out individually with each participant and consisted of an initial warm-up conversation. The interviews were audio-taped and subsequently transcribed. Their stories were analyzed based on their length, the causal network which measured overtly and non-overtly marked causal statements, causal chain events and causal connections. The findings of the study showed a developmental progress in producing longer stories and achieving narrative coherence by means of establishing causal relations between narrative events. The findings of the study showed that adults were significantly more likely to use overtly marked causal statements than children did; however, the difference between nine-year-olds and five-year-olds was not significant. These findings also showed that adults were significantly more likely to use non-overt statements than children and nine-year-olds outperformed five-year-olds in this regard. With regard to discourse tendency, within each group, the participants used significantly more non-overtly marked causal statements than the overtly marked statements. With regard to the encoding of causal-chain events, the findings showed that both nine-year-olds and five-year-olds outperformed adults by having larger density in this regard and despite that, the age-related difference between the two groups of children was not significant. The findings also showed that adults and nine-year-olds were significantly more likely to encode causal connections than were five-year-olds. With regard to the pattern of causal connectedness, the findings revealed age-related differences for each type. For C0, five-year-olds were significantly more likely to employ them than nine-year-olds and adults. A reverse pattern was shown for C3+ events, which were encoded more by both adults and nine-year-olds than by the younger children. While C1 events were the dominant type used by five-year-olds, C2 events were preferred by nine-year-olds. Therefore, nine-year-olds and adults tended to encode events with higher causal connectedness. The results of the study indicated that compared with causal connectives, the causal network appears to be a more sensitive tool to reveal children’s development in maintaining coherence. The results also showed that the system of causal network provides an alternative to quantitatively assess narrative coherence by covering all possible causal relations, which encompass overtly and non-overtly marked causal connectivity between adjacent and non-adjacent propositions. Since the narrative intelligence of participants was not controlled, it is suggested to control this variable in future research to obtain more accurate results. Researchers have pointed to the narrative intelligence as one of the cognitive determinants of narrative ability. They believe that factors related to intelligence play an important role in the development of the structure of narrative. Therefore, it is suggested to match participants in terms of narrative intelligence in order to obtain more reliable results.

Keywords

 
References
Afrooz Gh., & Kamkary, K. (2009). Psychometric principles and intelligence: Tehran University Stanford-Binan Intelligence Test (Persian). Tehran: Tehran University Press [In Persian].
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. InG. H. Bower(Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (pp. 47-89). New York: Academic Press.
Bamberg, M., & Damrad-Frye, R. (1991). On the ability to provide evaluative comments: Further explorations of children’s narrative competences. Journal of Child Language, 18, 689-710.
Bamberg, M., & Marchman, V. (1990). What holds a narrative together? The linguistic encoding of episode boundaries. Papers in Pragmatics, 4, 58-121.
Berman, R., & Slobin, D. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A cross linguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bringuier, J. (1896). Conversations with Jean Piaget. (M. Mahmoud & P. Dadsetan, Trans.). Tehran: SAMT [In Persian].
Chang, M., & Hsin-Yi, S. (2012). To mark or not to mark the cause, that is the question: Causal marking in Taiwanese conversation. Journal of Pragmatics, 44, 1743-1763.
Chaudron, C., & Richards, J. (1986). The effect of discourse markers on the comprehension of lectures. Applied Linguistics7(2),113-127.
Davis, A., Therese, O., & Coon, M. (1997). Referential cohesion and logical coherence of narration after right hemisphere stroke. Brain and Language, 56, 183-210.
Diehl, J., Bennetto, L. & Young, E. (2006). Story recall and narrative coherence of high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34(1), 87-102.
Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond ‘theory of mind’. Cognition, 50, 115-132.
Ghashmardi, M. & Rezanavaz, E. (2014). A textual criticism of the translations of the little prince according to textual linguistics. Language Research5(9), 175-201. doi: 10.22051/jlr.2014.1068 [In Persian].
Gathercole, S., Pickering, S., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177-190.
Grice, P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Habermas, T., & Bluck, S. (2000). Getting a life: The emergence of the life story in adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 748-769.
Hasanzadeh, S., & Minaei, A. (2002). Adaptation and standardization of the test of TOLD-P: 3 for Farsi-Speaking Children of Tehran. JOEC, 1(2),119-134.
Hammill, D., & Newcomer, P. (2008). The test of language development: Primary-4. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985). Language and cognitive processes from a developmental perspective. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1(1), 61-85.
Matthews, S., & Yip, V. (1994). Cantonese: A comprehensive grammar. London, United Kingdom: Routledge.
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, Where are you? New York: Dial Press.
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational Mind. Harvard, MA: MIT.
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child’s narrative. New York: Plenum.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1972). The psychology of the child. (H. Weaver, Trans.). New York: Basic Books.
Pishghadam, R., & Shams, M. A. (2013). A new look into the construct validity of the IELTS speaking module. The Journal of Teaching Language Skills (JTLS)5(1), 71-90 [In Persian].
Sah, W. (2007). A longitudinal investigation of Mandarin-speaking preschoolers’ relation of events in narratives: from unrelated to related events. Taiwan Journal of Linguistics, 5(1),77-96.
Sah, W., & Torng, P. (2012). Narrative coherence in high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorders: An investigation of causal relatedness. Paper presented at the 20th Annual Meeting of the International Association of Chinese Linguistics (28-31August, 2012), The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hung Hom, Hong Kong.
Schiffrin, D. (1987). Discourse markers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Snow, C. (1991). The theoretical basis for relationship between language and literacy in development. Journal of Research in Childhood Education6(1), 5-10.
Soleymani, Z., Nematzadeh, Sh., Gholami Tehrani, L., & Rahgozar, M. (2014). The reliability of language performance measurement in language sample analysis of children aged 5-6 years. Audiology, 23(1), 9-21 [In Persian].
Stein, N., & Albro, E. (1997). Building complexity and coherence: children’s use of goal-structured knowledge in telling stories. InM. Bamberg (Ed.), Narrative Development: Six Approaches (pp. 5-44). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Stein, N., & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In R. O. Freedle (Ed.), New Directions in Discourse Processing (pp.53-120). Norwood, NJ: ABLEX Publishing Corporation.
Trabasso, T., & Nickels, M. (1992). The development of goal plans of action in the narration of a picture story. Discourse Processes15, 249-275.
Trabasso, T., & Rodkin, P. (1994). Knowledge of goal/plans: A conceptual basis for narrating Frog, where are you? In R. Berman, and D. Slobin (Eds.), Relating Events in Narrative: A Crosslinguistic Developmental Study (pp.85-106). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Trabasso, T., Secco, T., & van den Broek, P. (1984). Causal cohesion and story coherence. In H. Mandl, N. L. Stein, and T. Trabasso (Eds.), Learning and Comprehension of Text (pp. 83-111). Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Trabasso, T., & Sperry, L. (1985). Causal relatedness and importance of story events. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 595-611.
Trabasso, T., Stein, N., Rodkin, P., Munger, M., & Baughn, C. (1992). Knowledge of goals and plans in the on-line narration of events. Cognitive Development, 7, 133-170.
Trabasso, T., & van den Broek, P. (1985). Causal thinking and the representation of narrative events. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 612-630.
Trabasso, T., van den Broek, P., & Young Suh, S. (1989). Logical necessity and transitivity of causal relations in stories. Discourse Processes, 12, 1-25.