Document Type : Research

Authors

1 PhD, Linguistics, Assistant Professor in Department of Language and Literature, Faculty member of Farhangian University, Tehran, Iran

2 PhD, Linguistics, Associate Professor in Linguistics Department, Faculty member of Allame Tabataba'i University, Tehran, Iran

Abstract

The role of semantic head and constituent position in processing and representation of root compound nouns comprised of noun-noun was studied in this article. For this purpose, the performance of three people (two men and one woman) with Broca aphasia was studied through naming and repetition of endocentric and exocentric root compound nouns taken from the Ph.D. dissertation of Xabbaz (2006). Easier processing of simple and compound nouns' repetition compared to their naming proved that repetition processing was different from naming. Moreover, easier processing of root compound nouns in comparison with simple ones showed the role that morphological structure plays in processing.
The occurrences of errors in one constituent of compound nouns (the initial or final constituent) and also in the whole compound (both initial and final constituents) confirmed that semantic head had no role in processing compound nouns. More errors of people with Broca aphasia in initial constituents, compared to the final constituents of endocentric (head-initial and head-final) and exocentric root compound nouns in confrontation naming, indicated that constituent position has a role in the processing of root compound nouns. Moreover, fewer errors of final constituents compared to initial constituents of compound nouns in repetition task provides an account of the easier processing of nouns' final constituents and supports the role of constituent position in the repetition process too. The distribution of errors in initial, final, and both constituents (the whole) of root compound nouns is an indication of their hierarchical and flat processing in naming and repetition tasks.
Analyzing the correct answers of people with aphasia in confrontation naming and repetition revealed that they had better performance in repetition than naming. The repetition of their simple nouns also was better than their naming. Moreover, they had better performance in repetition and naming of simple nouns in comparison with compound ones. The finding of easier processing of simple and compound nouns' repetition compared to their naming processing proved that auditory repetition and confrontation naming were processed differently. Moreover, easier processing of simple nouns compared to compound ones indicated that the structure of words played an important role in processing of words. In other words, words with complex structures are processed more difficultly than words whose structures are simple.
Regarding the fact that the head and its position are separate from each other in exocentric root compound nouns and none of the constituents are heads; this independence shows the role of constituent position in both hierarchical and flat processing of compound nouns. If the head finial compound nouns, whose heads are located in words' final, have an easier processing, it is expected that the exocentric nouns will be processed in the same way. In other words, since none of the constituents of exocentric compound nouns are heads, it is expected that both constituents will be processed at the same degree of easiness, but contrary to this expectation, analyzing the errors which are produced by people with Broca aphasias showed that the distribution of errors were not different from endocentric compound nouns. This means that although none of constituents of exocentric compound nouns are heads, like endocentric ones, their final heads are processed more easily
Moreover, the fewer errors of these constituents show the important role of constituent positions in processing. In addition, this finding displays the role of compound nouns' final heads too. It also reveals that the processing of exocentric and endocentric compound nouns are the same. The distribution of errors in initial and final, or both constituents of compound nouns simultaneously shows the hierarchical and flat processing of confrontation naming. On the other hand, the higher number of occurrences of errors in both constituents of root compound nouns demonstrates that the flat processing is more frequent than the hierarchical one in naming these stimuli. This finding is in accordance with what Marelli, Crepaldi & Luzzatti (2009) found in processing of compound words. The distribution of errors in initial, and final or both constituents of compound nouns produced by people with aphasia proves that both the flat and hierarchical processing occur in repetition. Moreover, the higher occurrences of errors in initial and final head constituents of compound nouns compared to the whole compounds (both constituents) of these nouns shows that the frequent processing is hierarchical in repetition. This finding is in accordance with what El Yagoubi, Chiarelli, Mondini, Perrone, Danieli, & Semenza (2008) claimed on potential impact of headedness in processing of nouns in Italian.

Keywords


References
Abolhasanizadeh, V., Bijankhan, M., & Gussenhoven, C. (2012). The Persian pitch accent and its retention after the focus. Lingua, 122, 1380-1394.
Andrews, S., Miller, B., & Rayner, K. (2004). Eye movements and morphological segmentation of compound words: There is a mouse in mousetrap. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 285–311.
Anvari, H, (2007). Farhang-i buzurg-i Sukhan, Tehran: Soxan Pbulication [In Persian].
Arcara, G., Marelli, M., Buodo, G., & Mondini, S. (2014). Compound headedness in the mental lexicon: An event-related potential study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 31(1-2), 164-183.
Badecker, W. (2001). Lexical composition and the production of compounds: Evidence from errors in naming. Lang. Cogn. Processes, 16, 337–366. doi: 10.1080/01690960042000120
Bakhtiar, M., Nilipour, N., & Weekes, B. S. (2013). Predictors of timed picture naming in Persian. Behavior Research, 45, 834-841. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0298-6
Barry, c., Morrison, C. M., & Ellis, A.W. (1997). Naming the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures: Effects of age of acquisition, frequency and name agreement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 560-585.
Bertram, R., & Hyona, J. (2003). The length of a complex word modifies the role of morphological structure: Evidence from eye movements when reading short and long Finnish compounds. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 615–634.
Bien, H., Levelt, W., & Baayen, R. (2005). Frequency effects in compound production. PNAS, 102, 17876–17881.
Butterworth, B. (1983). Lexical representation. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production (pp. 257-294). New York: Academic Press.
Bijankhan, M., Sheykhzadegan, J., Bahrani, M., & Ghayoomi, M. (2011). Lessons from building a Persian written corpus: Peykare. Language Resources and Evaluation, 45, 143–164. doi:10.1007/ s10579-010-9132-x.
Butterworth, B. (1983). Lexical representation. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production (pp. 257-294). New York: Academic Press.
Bybee, J. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10 (5), 425-455.
Chiarelli, V., Menichelli, A., & Semenza, C. (2007). Naming compounds in Alzhemer’s disease. The Mental Lexicon, 2, 259-269.
Cuetos, F., Ellis, A. W., & Alvarez, B. (1999). Naming times for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31, 650–658. doi:10.3758/BF03200741
Dressler, W. U. (2006). Compound types. In G. Libben & G. Jarema (Eds.), The representation and processing of compound words (pp. 23-44). Oxford, UK: OxfordUniversity Press.
Dunabeitia, J. A., Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (2007). The role of the frequency of constituents in compound words: Evidence from Basque and Spanish. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(6), 1171–1176.
El Yagoubi, R., Chiarelli, V., Mondini, S., Perrone, G., Danieli, M., & Semenza, C. (2008). Neural correlates of Italian nominal compounds and potential impact of headedness effect: An ERP study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 559-581.
Fabb, N. (1998). Compounding. In A. Spencer & A. M. Zwicky (Eds.), The handbook of morphology (pp. 690-706.). Oxford/Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Ghonchepour, M., & Pakzad Moghaddam, M. (2019). The role of semantic transparency in processing compound nouns: Evidence from people with Broca’s aphasia. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics,1-23. doi: 10.1080/02699206.2019.1656780
Ghonchepour, M. (2014) Verbal compounding in Persian (PhD dissertation). Allame Tabataba'I University, Tehran, Iran [In Persian].
Ghonchepour, M. (2018). Verbal compounding: A syntactic or Morphological processing? A corpus- based study. Journal of Language Research, 10(28), 149-173 [In Persian].
Ghonchepour, M., & Raghibdoust, Sh. (2012). Production of simple and compound nouns in Persian Ahasics. Journal of Language Research, 5, 177-194 [In Persian].
Ghonchepour, M., Kalantari khandani, E., & Farrokhi Barfe, M. H. (2019). Semantic transparency and compound nouns processing: Evidence from people with aphasia. Journal of Research in Linguistics, 2, 1-24, [In Persian].
Goldman, R. (2007). Compounding in Aphasia: A crosslinguistic review (BA thesis). Swarthmore College, Department of Linguistics, Swartmore, PA. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/10066/10186
Gunter, T., Friederici, A., & Schriefers, H. (2000). Syntactic gender and semantic expectancy: ERPs reveal early autonomy and late interaction. J. Cogn. Neurosci,12, 556–568. doi: 10.1162/089892900562336
Haspelmath, M. (2002). Understanding morphology. London: Macmillan Press LTD.
Isel, F., Gunter, T. C., & Friederici, A. D. (2003). Prosody-assisted head-driven access to spoken German compounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(2), 277.
Jackson-Maldonado D., & Conboy B. T. (2007). Utterance length measures for Spanish-speaking toddlers: The morpheme vs. word issue re-visited. In J. G. Centeno, R. Anderson & L. K. Obler (Eds.), Communication disorders in Spanish speakers: Theoretical, research and clinical aspects (pp. 142–155). Clevedon, United Kingdom: Multilingual Matters.
Janssen, N., Bi, Y., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A tale of two frequencies: Determining the speed of lexical access for Mandarin Chinese and English compounds. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 1191–1223.
Jarema, G., Busson, C., Nikolova, R., Tsapkini, K., & Libben, G. (1999). Processing compounds: A cross-linguistic study. Brain and Language, 68, 362–369.
Jarema, G., Perlak, D., & Semenza, C. (2009). The processing of compounds in bilingual aphasia. Aphasiology, 24, 126–140.
Katamba, F.,& Stonhan, J. (2006). Morphology. England: Macmillan Press LTD.
Koester, D., Gunter, T. C., & Wagner, S. (2007). The morphosyntactic decomposition and semantic composition of German compound words investigated by ERPs. Brain and Language, 102, 64–79.
Koester, D., Gunter, T. C., Wagner, S., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Morphosyntax, prosody, and linking elements: The auditory processing of German nominal compounds. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1647–1668. doi: 10.1162/0898929042568541
Koester, D., Holle, H., & Gunter, T. C. (2009). Electrophysiological evidence for incremental lexical–semantic integration in auditory compound comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1854–1864.
Kuperman, V., Schreuder, R., Bertram, R., & Baayen, R.H. (2009). Reading of multimorphemic Dutch compounds: Towards a multiple route model of lexical processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 876-895.
Lemhöfer, K., Koester, D., & Schreuder, R. (2011). When bicycle pump is harder to read than bicycle bell: Effects of parsing cues in first and second language compound reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 364–370.
Lieber, R. (2009). A lexical semantic approach to compounding. In R. Lieber., & P. štekauer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of compounding (pp. 78-104). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lieber, R. (2010). Introducing morphology. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Lima, S. D., & Pollatsek, A. (1983). Lexical access via an orthographic code? The basic orthographic syllabic structure (BOSS) reconsidered. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 22(3), 310-332. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(83)90215-3.
Marelli, M., & Luzzatti, C. (2012). Frequency effects in the processing of Italian nominal compounds: Modulation of headedness and semantic transparency. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 644–664.
Marelli, M., Aggujaro, S., Molteni, F., & Luzzatti, C. (2012). The multiple-lemma representation of Italian compound nouns: A single case study of deep dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 50, 852–861.
Marelli, M., Crepaldi, D., & Luzzatti, C. (2009). Head position and the mental representation of nominal compounds: A constituent priming study in Italian.The Mental Lexicon, 4, 430–454.
Marelli, M., Giusy Zonca, G., Antonella Contardi, A., & Claudio Luzzatti, C. (2014). The representation of compound headedness in the mental lexicon: A picture naming study in aphasia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 31, 26-39. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2013.860024
McKinnon, R., Allen, M., & Osterhout, L. (2003). Morphological decomposition involving no productive morphemes: ERP Evidence. NeuroReport, 14, 883-886.
Mondini, S., Jarema, G., Luzzatti, C., Burani, C., & Semenza, C. (2002). Why is “Red Cross” different from “Yellow Cross”? A neuropsychological study of noun–adjective agreement within Italian compounds. Brain and Language, 81, 621–634.
Mondini, S., Luzzatti, C., Zonca, G., Pistarini, C., & Semenza, C. (2004). The mental representation of verb-noun compounds in Italian: Evidence from a multiple single-case study in aphasia. Brain and Language, 90, 470–477.
Nilipour, R. (1994). Diagnostic aphasia test. Tehran: Iran University of Medical Sciences Press [In Persian].
Nilipour, R., Bakhtiar, M., Momenian, M., & Weekes, B.s. (2016). Object and action picture naming in brain-damaged Persian speakers with aphasia. Aphasiology, 31 (4), 388-40. DOI: 10.1080/02687038.2016.1234583
Nippold, M. A.,& Christine, H. M. (2013). Spoken language production in adults: Examining age-related differences in syntactic complexity. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 28(3), 195-207. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2013.841292
Parker M. D., Brorson K. (2005). A comparative study between mean length of utterance in morphemes (MLUm) and mean length of utterance in words (MLUw). First Lang. 25, 365–376. 10.1177/0142723705059114
Rahmani, H., Rietveld, T., & Gussenhoven, C. (2015). Stress “deafness” reveals absence of lexical marking of stress or tone in the adult grammar. PLOS ONE. 10 (12), e0143968. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0143968
Sandra, D. (1990). On the representation and processing of compound words: Automatic access to constituent morphemes does not occur. Quarterly Journal of Expremental Psychology. 42, 529-567.
Semenza, C., & Mondini, S. (2006). Neuropsychology of compound words. In G. Libben & G. Jarema (Eds.), The representation and processing of compound words (pp. 71–95). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Semenza, C., Arcara, G., Facchini, S., Meneghello, F., Ferraro, M., Passarini, L., & Mondini, S. (2011). Reading compounds in neglect dyslexia: The headedness effect. Neuropychologia, 49, 3116–3120.
Severens, E., Van Lommel, S., Ratinckx, E., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2005). Timed picture naming norms for 590 pictures in Dutch. Acta Psychologica, 119, 159–187. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.01.002
Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6(2), 174-215.
Stockall, L., & Marantz, A. (2006). A single route, full decomposition model of morphological complexity: MEG evidence. The Mental Lexicon, 1(1), 85-123.
Taft, M., & Ardasinski (2006). Obligatory decomposition in reading prefixed words. The Mental Lexicon, 1, 183-199.
Taft, M. (2004). Morphological decomposition and the reverse base frequency effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57 (4), 745–765.
Taft, M. (2006). A localist-cum-distributed (LCD) framework for lexical processing. In S.M. Andrews (Ed.), From ink marks to ideas: Current issues in lexical processing (pp. 76-94). Hove, England: Psychology Press.
Taft, M., & Forster, K. I. (1976). Lexical storage and retrieval of polymorphemic and poly-syllabic words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal behavior, 15, 607-620.
Xabbaz, M, (2006). Root compounding in Persian (PhD dissertation), Allame Tabataba'I University, Tehran, Iran [In Persian].
Zwitserlood, P. (1994). The role of semantic transparency in the processing and representation of Dutch compounds. Language and Cognitive Processes. 9(3), 341-368